The brainstem oscillator for whisking and the case for breathing as the master clock for orofacial motor actions In Cold

نویسندگان

  • David Kleinfeld
  • Jeffrey D. Moore
  • Fan Wang
  • Martin Deschênes
چکیده

170 words Narrative 3250 words Legends: 1870 words References: 54 Figures 8 all CMYK and to final size Editorial correspondence: David Kleinfeld Department of Physics 0374 University of California San Diego 9500 Gilman Drive La Jolla, CA 92093 Office: 858-822-0342 Mobile: 858-922-4664 Fax: 858-534-7697 Email: [email protected] Final draft: 2 January 2015 2 Abstract Whisking and sniffing are predominant aspects of exploratory behavior in rodents. We review evidence that these motor rhythms are coordinated by the respiratory patterning circuitry in the ventral medulla. A recently described region in the intermediate reticular zone of the medulla functions as an autonomous whisking oscillator, whose neuronal output is reset upon each breath by input from the preBötzinger complex. Based on similarities between this neuronal circuit architecture and that of other orofacial behaviors, we propose that the preBötzinger complex, which projects broadly to premotor regions throughout the intermediate reticular zone of the medulla, functions as a master clock to coordinate multiple orofacial actions involved in exploratory and ingestive behaviors. We then extend the analysis of whisking to the relatively slow control of the midpoint of the whisk. We conjecture, in a manner consistent with breathing as the "master clock" for all orofacial behaviors, that slow control optimizes the position of sensors while the breathing rhythms provides a means to perceptually bind the inputs from different orofacial modalities. Final draft: 2 January 2015 3 A postulate from the neuroethology movement is the presence of a hierarchical control structure for motor acts that is orchestrated by the nervous system (Weiss 1941; Tinbergen 1951). At the base of this hierarchy are individual motor “actions”. For example, orofacial actions that comprise sensory exploration would correspond to a sniff, a twitch of the nose, a lick, a whisk, or a turn of the head. These actions are coordinated in nested groups to form more complex “behaviors”. The nature of the interactions among the neuronal circuits that generate these actions and coordinate their output is a foundational topic in computational neuroscience. One question, which motivates the present work, concerns the possibility of using a master clock as a means to organize motor actions into behaviors. The use of such a clock could provide a computationally simple mechanism to avoid antagonistic movements and to adhere to biomechanical constraints. Presumably, the clock could function as part of a larger circuit that involves interactions between motor and premotor neuronal centers, as well as environmental and proprioceptive feedback signals. For the case of exploratory movements, the use of a common clock to actively sample the sensory environment could simplify the problem of binding inputs arising from different modalities, e.g., smell, touch, and taste (Kleinfeld et al. 2014). The notion of a master clock is implicit in the pioneering work of Welker (1964), published a half century ago, who observed that movement of the nose, the head, and the vibrissae of rodents appear to be coherent with rapid breathing, or sniffing. Together, these three actions are hypothesized to synchronize with the breathing cycle to form a stereotyped exploratory behavior that rodents use to rhythmically sample their surrounding olfactory and tactile environments (Fig. 1). A parsimonious conclusion is that the breathing cycle may be the master clock at the root of all rhythmic orofacial behaviors. Here we summarize our recent work on the neuronal circuitry that controls rhythmic exploratory whisking by rats (Moore and Deschênes et al. 2013); this work provides evidence that whisking is subsumed by the breathing rhythm. Our summary is followed by a presentation of two threads of subsequent, and ongoing, inquiry. Motivated by anatomical similarities between this newly-discovered brainstem whisking circuit and those circuits thought to control the tongue and jaw, the first thread bears Final draft: 2 January 2015 4 directly on the issue of a master clock and concerns the potential role of breathing in the synchronization of other motor actions. The second thread addresses the mechanistic basis for controlling the slowly-varying amplitude and position of whisking, which we propose may be organized similarly to the control of body posture. It is instructive to begin with an overview of the motor plant for whisking. A Manhattan-like grid of follicles is embedded within the mystacial pad (Fig. 2), which forms the cheek. Intrinsic muscles wrap around individual vibrissa follicles and are only partially inserted into the skin (Fig. 2A). These muscles resemble slings that embrace the lower half of the follicle, and they are anchored to each of two adjacent follicles of the same row (Dorfl 1982; Wineski 1985) (Fig. 2B). In addition, five superficial extrinsic muscles are inserted into either the plate or the skin of the pad, between the rows of follicles. These muscles include M. nasalis as a protractor and M. nasolabialis and M. maxillolabialis as retractors (Fig. 2C). Simultaneous measurements of the electromyogram (EMG) from the intrinsic and extrinsic muscles indicate the presence of three phases of muscle contraction during the whisking cycle (Fig. 2D), with different phases for the extrinsic protractor, intrinsic protractor, and extrinsic retractor muscles (Hill et al. 2008). To verify our understanding of the muscular control of whisking, we constructed a mechanical model of the motor plant to predict the motion of the vibrissae and the mystacial pad from the measured EMGs of the intrinsic and different extrinsic muscles (Hill et al. 2008) (Fig. 2D). The anatomy of the musculature, including attachment points in the skin and plate together with measurements of the viscoelastic restoring forces, provide the data that informs the model (Hill et al. 2008; Simony et al. 2010) (Fig. 2E). For whisking while sniffing, we find excellent agreement between the calculated and measured motion of both the vibrissae and the pad (Fig. 2F). This implies the presence of a multi-phase rhythmic pattern generator. For concreteness, we focus this presentation on the rhythm that dominates protraction through direct drive of the intrinsic muscles. Such a presentation is analogous to much of the literature that describes the motor control of breathing, which also consists of three phases of neuronal activity, yet the inspiratory phase can be driven by a single oscillator circuit (Smith et al. 2009). Final draft: 2 January 2015 5 Concurrent measurements of breathing and whisking in head-restrained rats reveal key aspects of their coordination (Moore and Deschênes et al. 2013; Ranade et al. 2013), and extend Welker’s initial descriptions (Fig. 3). First, breathing over a wide range of frequencies can occur without substantial whisking (Fig. 3A). To determine whether whisking can also occur without breathing, a puff of ammonia was applied to the snout to inactivate the central inspiratory drive (Lawson et al. 1991) and temporarily inhibit respiration. Critically, rats can whisk during such a disruption in breathing (Fig. 3B). These data imply that there are separate, or separable, oscillator circuits for breathing and whisking. Sniffing is always accompanied by a one-to-one relation with whisking (Fig. 3A,B), as first noted by Welker (1964). Yet basal breathing, which is twoto fivetimes slower than whisking, is accompanied by two forms of whisks: those that are coincident with an inspiration, which we denote "inspiratory-locked whisks", and those that occur between successive breaths and successively diminish in amplitude, which we denote "intervening whisks". (Fig. 3C,D). The "intervening whisks" are incommensurate with breathing. These data, with further support from phase-response curves (Moore and Deschênes et al. 2013), imply that the oscillator circuit for breathing resets the phase of the oscillator for whisking. Where is the pattern generator for this fast, rhythmic vibrissa motion? One possibility is that rapid whisking is generated by a disynaptic feedback loop in the brainstem (Nguyen and Kleinfeld 2005; Deutsch et al. 2012; Matthews et al. 2015). That is, sensory signals generated by the motion of the vibrissa could drive the facial motoneurons, which in turn would regenerate a sensory signal, and so on. However, lesion of the vibrissa sensory nerve, i.e., the infraorbial branch of the trigeminal, has minimal effect on whisking (Welker 1964; Gao et al. 2001; Berg and Kleinfeld 2003) and thus disproves this hypothesis. Further, lesion of any of a large number of midbrain and forebrain nuclei only minimally disrupts whisking (Welker 1964; Lovick 1972; Semba and Komisaruk 1984). These observations lead to the alternative hypothesis that there is an independent oscillator that drives whisking and is located in the brainstem. But where? Given the close phase relationship between whisking and breathing, the neighborhood of the ventral respiratory column in the medulla (Smith et al. 2009) is a Final draft: 2 January 2015 6 plausible suspect. A role for breathing in the control of whisking is also suggested by the shared facial musculature between the two behaviors (Sherrey and Megirian 1977; Haidarliu et al. 2012). We thus directed our search near the location of the preBötzinger complex, which was first demonstrated by Feldman and colleagues (Smith et al. 1991; Tan et al. 2008) to generate the inspiratory breathing rhythm. The close, one-to-one relationship between sniffing and whisking makes it difficult to discriminate between potential breathing versus whisking neuronal centers (Fig 3A). However, the difference in the frequency of basal respiration versus whisking provides a behavioral signature for such a discrimination (Moore and Deschênes et al. 2013) (Fig. 3C,D). We recorded unit spiking activity within the preBötzinger complex as well as in the rostral ventral respiratory group in head-fixed awake rats. As expected, spiking occurred in phase with inspiration and with vibrissa protraction during inspiratory-locked whisks (Fig. 3C and red dots in Fig. 3E). Critically, the spiking activity did not track the intervening whisks. In contrast, we located a subset of units in the intermediate band of the reticular formation (IRt) whose spiking was synchronous with the protraction of both inspiratory-locked and intervening whisks (Fig. 3D and blue dots in Fig. 3E). These units are potential premotor inputs to the motoneurons that drive the intrinsic muscles that protract the vibrissae (Fig. 2A,B). They are located in the ventral part of the IRt, medial to the ambiguus nucleus and immediately dorsomedial to the preBötzinger complex. We denote this new region the vibrissa zone of the IRt (vIRt) and refer to units in this region as "whisking units". To demonstrate the role of the vIRt in the generation of whisking, we address the necessity and sufficiency of this region for rhythmic vibrissa protraction. In support of necessity, we observe that small lesions of the vIRt abolish whisking on the ipsilateral side of the lesion, while whisking persists on the contralateral side (Fig. 4A). Off-site lesions, on the other hand, have minimal effects on whisking (Fig. 4B). Sufficiency of the vIRt for rhythmic motion was assessed by activation of this region through microinjection of the glutamate receptor agonist kainic acid in the vicinity of the vIRt. We observe prolonged rhythmic vibrissa movements, at normal whisking frequencies (Carvell and Simons 1990; Berg and Kleinfeld 2003), for injections in the lightly anesthetized rat (Fig. 4C) and mouse (Moore et al. 2014a). The frequency of whisking Final draft: 2 January 2015 7 that is induced by kainic acid is observed to decrease over time, while the frequency of basal breathing remains at a constant rate. These data imply that the chemical activation is sufficiently strong to temporally unlock rhythmic vibrissa protraction from breathing. The ability to induce whisking in an immobile animal through kainic acid microinjection provides a means to stably record from units whose firing times are coherent with rhythmic protraction. We identified neuronal units that spike in synchrony with protraction, as in the case of the units identified during intervening whisks in the behaving animal (Fig. 3D), as well as units that spike in anti-phase (Fig. 4D). This finding is suggestive of a rhythm that results from local network dynamics as opposed to coupled single-cell oscillators (Getting 1989; Kleinfeld and Sompolinsky 1989). The behavioral data (Fig. 3) implies that neurons in the preBötzinger complex reset an oscillatory network of whisking units in the vIRt that initiate protraction of the vibrissae concurrent with each inspiration. What is the nature of the connectivity from the preBotzinger complex to the vIRt? We assessed the underlying connectivity for this action by tract tracing methods (Moore and Deschênes et al.) (Fig. 5A). Injections of an anterograde tracer into the preBötzinger complex, identified by the phase relation of units relative to breathing (Fig. 5B), led to dense anterograde labeling of terminals in the same vIRt region where we observed whisking units and where lesions extinguished ipsilateral whisking (Fig. 5C). These results support a direct connection from the preBötzinger complex to the vIRt. In addition, injection of anterograde tracer into the region of the vIRt does not lead to discernable labeling in the preBötzinger complex (unpublished observations). These data imply that there is only unilateral connectivity from the breathing oscillator to the whisking oscillator, consistent with the reset of whisking by breathing but not vice versa (Moore and Deschênes et al. 2013). We next delineated the projections from neurons in the vIRt to facial motoneurons. Here we used a retrograde scheme based on replication incompetent rabies virus to label from the muscles in the mystacial pad transynaptically back to the vIRt. A modified rabies virus, denoted ∆G-RV, in which the gene for glycoprotein, G, was deleted and the gene for red fluorescent protein inserted, was grown in cells engineered to produce G (Wickersham et al. 2007). This virus is injected into the facial Final draft: 2 January 2015 8 musculature of juvenile transgenic mice that express G in cholinergic neurons, so that infected motoneurons can recapitulated active rabies for subsequent labeling of premotoneurons. We observe strong labeling in the location of the vIRt (Fig. 5D) (Takatoh et al. 2013). These and previous (Isokawa-Akesson and Komisaruk 1987) patterns of neuronal labeling in the IRt support a direct connection from the vIRt to the facial nucleus and substantiate the role of the vIRt as the premotor pattern generator for rhythmic whisking. In toto, the vIRt is an element of a larger circuit whereby cells in or near the preBötzinger complex for inspiration (Tan et al. 2008; Bouvier et al. 2010; Gray et al. 2010) reset the phase of vIRt units with each breath during exploratory whisking (Fig. 5E). Does the pathway found for the reset of whisking by breathing generalize to the coordination of other orfacial behaviors by breathing (Fig. 1)? From a purely anatomical basis, Feldman and colleagues (Tan et al. 2010) showed that neurons that are critical to the generation of the inspiratory rhythm in the preBötzinger complex extend collateral projections across the ventral to dorsal extent of the IRt (Fig. 6A). These axons are likely to intersect the dendritic fields of premotor nuclei, like that of the vIRt for whisking, that drive other rhythmic orofacial actions (Fig. 6B). The best described of these is licking, for which tongue protrusions are coordinated with the respiratory cycle (Welzl and Bures 1977), like whisking, may be controlled by a separate oscillator (Wiesenfeld et al. 1977). A cluster of hypoglossal premotoneurons are concentrated dorsomedial to the preBötzinger complex within the IRt (Travers et al. 1997; Koizumi et al. 2008; Stanek et al. 2014) (Fig. 6B). The spiking output of units in this region of the IRt locks to rhythmic licking (Travers et al. 2000), while infusion of an inhibitory agonist into the IRt blocks licking (Chen et al. 2001). These observations provide evidence in support of sufficiency and necessity, respectively, for these units a premotor rhythm generator. Critically, hypoglossal premotoneurons produce bursts of spikes that are locked to inspiration (Ono et al. 1998). This evidence is therefore consistent with parallel premotor patterning circuits for licking and whisking in adjacent sub-compartments of the IRt that are both influenced by breathing-related neurons in the preBotzinger complex. Beyond whisking and licking, the putative premotor oscillators for chewing, i.e., the oral portion of the IRt (Travers et al. 2005; 2010), for control of the airway, i.e., the Final draft: 2 January 2015 9 ambiguus nucleus (Bieger and Hopkins 1987), and for licking, i.e., the hypoglossal IRt dorsomedial to the vIRt, are located proximally to each other (Travers et al. 1997) and to the preBötzinger complex (Fig. 6B). All told, the existing results are consistent with a model in which preBötzinger units reset the onset of bursts in a network of hypoglossal premotor neurons in the IRt zone, and reset or otherwise influence the spiking of premotor neurons for other orofacial actions in parallel with our model of the circuit for whisking (Fig. 5E). Our ongoing work addresses the formation of behaviors through the coordinated integration of these disparate orofacial motor actions (Moore et al. 2014b). We now step back and recall that rhythmic motor actions depend on the envelope of the rhythmic drive as well as the timing of the underlying oscillations. For the case of whisking, the envelope consists of a slowly varying amplitude, which may also gate the rhythm, and a slowly varying midpoint that sets the center of interest for exploratory whisks (Carvell et al. 1991; Berg and Kleinfeld 2003; Hill et al. 2008) (Fig. 7A). A priori, the amplitude of whisking could be controlled by the vIRt or by a separate premotor nucleus that targets vibrissa motoneurons in the facial motor nucleus. The initial evidence for the latter possibility comes from a collaboration between the Ahissar and Vogel groups (Pietr et al. 2010), where it was shown that the normal variations in the amplitude of whisking are suppressed by systemic administration of an antagonist of the cannabinoid receptor but heightened by an agonist (Fig. 7B,C). In contrast, there is no change on the frequency of the whisk cycle (Fig. 7B,D). This result implies that the control of the slowly varying amplitude of whisking and the rapidly varying phase are independent, although the locus of the control of amplitude remains unknown. The independence of midpoint and the whisking rhythm was established by simultaneously monitoring the position of the vibrissae on both sides of the face before and after lesion of the vIRt in alert, head-restrained rats. In new data, we observe approximate synchronous motion of the midpoint between the right and left sides of the face during bouts of rhythmic whisking and stability of the midpoint between bouts (red lines in Fig. 7E). Lesion of the vIRt on one side abolished rhythmic whisking on that side, yet concurrent changes in the midpoint between the right and left sides of the face is maintained (Fig. 7F). This result implies the existence of a yet to be determined Final draft: 2 January 2015 10 brainstem nucleus that controls slow changes in vibrissae position, potentially under the command of descending projections from other brain areas. From a mathematical perspective, the independence of the slowly varying envelope of whisking from rapid changes in phase suggests that the angle of vibrissae relative to the face, θ(t), may be expressed as θ(t) = θamp(t)•cos [φ(t)] + θmid(t) with dφ(t)/dt = 2πfwhisk, where fwhisk is the instantaneous whisking frequency (Hill et al. 2011), and θamp(t) and θmid(t) are the aforementioned slowly-varying amplitude and midpoint parameters. How are these slowly varying parameters coded in the brain, and how do they influence the facial motoneurons that drive whisking? The first of these questions was examined by recording unit activity in agranular motor cortex (Brecht et al. 2004), concurrent with measurements of vibrissa position (Fig. 8A), as animals whisked in air (Hill et al. 2011; Friedman et al. 2012) (Fig. 8B). We observe that the firing patterns of neurons in motor cortex preferentially report the slowly varying amplitude and midpoint of a whisking bout (Fig. 8C). Across a population of neurons, the modulation is approximately linear in the basal spike rate, with both increases and decreases in rate across different units for the same change in the envelope of whisking (Fig. 8D). One possible explanation for how these signals may influence facial motoneurons, under current investigation, is that they act as commands that are relayed through trigeminal nuclei (Matthews et al. 2015) that further project to facial motor neurons (Pinganaud et al. 1999). It will be valuable to determine if similar neuronal coding applies to the cortical control of other orofacial actions (Sawczuk and Mosier 2001). How does the motion of a "master clock" play out with cognition? Let us suppose, consistent with the anatomy and the case for whisking and most likely for licking, that the relatively tight spatial organization of orofacial oscillators in the intermediate reticular zone provides a means for their reset by breathing as well as their rapid interaction (Fig. 6A). Then every orofacial sense, be it touch through whisking, smell through sniffing, and taste and touch through licking, will have a common underlying temporal signature through breathing. Thus the breathing rhythm, in addition to its physiological role in homeostasis, may serve a perceptual role in the binding of orofacial sensory inputs that enter through the pons, medulla and olfactory bulb. These sensory inputs converge at the level of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Kleinfeld et al. 2014), a site Final draft: 2 January 2015 11 of decision making (Rushworth et al. 2007) that presumably incorporates the converge of evidence from multiple sensory streams. It will be of interest to determine if binding by the respiratory rhythm plays a role in the spiking patterns of units in this high-order brain region, and in their influence on motor output. Acknowledgements. Our research was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (grant MT-5877), the United States National Institutes of Health (grants NS058668, NS066664, NS047101 and NS077986), the United States National Science Foundation (grant PHY-1451026), and the US-Israeli Binational Science Foundation (grant 2003222). Final draft: 2 January 2015 12 References Berg RW, Kleinfeld D. 2003. Rhythmic whisking by rat: Retraction as well as protraction of the vibrissae is under active muscular control. Journal of Neurophysiology 89: 104-117. Bieger D, Hopkins DA. 1987. Viscerotopic representation of the upper alimentary tract in the medulla oblongata in the rat: The nucleus ambiguus. Journal of Comparative Neurology 262: 546-562. Bouvier J, Thoby-Brisson M, N. R, Dubreuil V, Ericson J, Champagnat J, Pierani A, A. C, Fortin G. 2010. Hindbrain interneurons and axon guidance signaling critical for breathing. Nature Neuroscience 13: 1066-1074. Brecht M, Krauss A, Muhammad S, Sinai-Esfahani L, Bellanca S, Margrie TW. 2004. Organization of rat vibrissa motor cortex and adjacent areas according to cytoarchitectonics, microstimulation, and intracellular stimulation of identified cells. Jounal of Comparative Neurology 479: 360-373. Carvell GE, Simons DJ. 1990. Biometric analyses of vibrissal tactile discrimination in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience 10: 2638-2648. Carvell GE, Simons DJ, Lichtenstein SH, Bryant P. 1991. Electromyographic activity of mystacial pad musculature during whisking behavior in the rat. Somatosensory and Motor Research 8: 159-164. Chen Z, Travers SP, Travers JB. 2001. Muscimol infusions in the brain stem reticular formation reversibly block ingestion in the awake rat. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Intergrative, and Comparative Physiology 280: R1085-1094. Deutsch D, Pietr M, Knutsen PM, Ahissar E, Schneidman E. 2012. Fast feedback in active sensing: touch-induced changes to whisker-object interaction. Public Library of Science ONE 7: e44272. Dorfl J. 1982. The musculature of the mystacial vibrissae of the white mouse. Journal of Anatomy 135: 147-154. Friedman WA, Zeigler HP, Keller A. 2012. Vibrissae motor cortex unit activity during whisking. Journal of Neurophysiology 107: 551-563. Gao P, Bermejo R, Zeigler HP. 2001. Vibrissa deaffentation and rodent whisking patterns: Behavioral evidence for a central pattern generator. Journal of Neuroscience 21: 53745380. Getting PA. 1989. Emerging principles governing the operation of neural networks. Annual Review of Neuroscience 12: 185-204. Gray PA, Hayes JA, Ling GY, Llona I, Tupal S, Picardo MC, Ross SE, Hirata T, Corbin JG, Eugenín J et al. 2010. Developmental origin of preBötzinger complex respiratory neurons. Journal of Neuroscience 30: 14883-148895. Haidarliu S, Golomb D, Kleinfeld D, Ahissar E. 2012. Dorsorostral snout muscles in the rat subserve coordinated movement for whisking and sniffing. Anatomical Record 295: 1181-1191. Haidarliu S, Simony E, Golomb D, Ahissar E. 2010. Muscle architecture in the mystacial pad of the rat. Anatomical Record 293: 1192–1206. Hill DN, Bermejo R, Zeigler HP, Kleinfeld D. 2008. Biomechanics of the vibrissa motor plant in rat: Rhythmic whisking consists of triphasic neuromuscular activity. Journal of Neuroscience 28: 3438-3455. Hill DN, Curtis JC, Moore JD, Kleinfeld D. 2011. Primary motor cortex reports efferent control of vibrissa position on multiple time scales. Neuron 72: 344–356. Final draft: 2 January 2015 13 Isokawa-Akesson M, Komisaruk BR. 1987. Difference in projections to the lateral and medial facial nucleus: Anatomically separate pathways for rhythmical vibrissa movement in rats. Experimental Brain Research 65: 385-398. Kleinfeld D, Deschênes M, Wang F, Moore JD. 2014. More than a rhythm of life: Breathing as a binder of orofacial sensation. Nature Neurocience 15: 647-651. Kleinfeld D, Sompolinsky H. 1989. Associative network models for central pattern generators. in Methods in Neuronal Modeling: From Synapses to Networks (eds. C Koch, I Segev), pp. 195-246. MIT Press, Campridge. Koizumi H, Wilson CG, Wong S, Yamanishi T, Koshiya N, Smith JC. 2008. Functional imaging, spatial reconstruction, and biophysical analysis of a respiratory motor circuit isolated in vitro. Journal of Neuroscience 28: :2353–2365. Lawson EE, Richter DW, Czyzyk-Krzeska MF, Bischoff A, Rudesill RC. 1991. Respiratory neuronal activity during apnea and other breathing patterns induced by laryngeal stimulation. Journal of Applied Ohysiology 70: 2742-2749. Lovick TA. 1972. The behavioural repertoire of precollicular decerebrate rats. Journal of Physiology 226: 4P-6P. Matthews DW, Deschênes M, Furuta T, Moore JD, Wang F, Karten HJ, Kleinfeld D. 2015. Feedback in the brainstem: An excitatory disynaptic pathway for control of whisking. Journal of Comparative Neurology: in press. Moore JD, Deschênes M, Kurnikova A, Kleinfeld D. 2014a. Activation and measurement of free whisking in the lightly anesthetized rodent. Nature Protocols 9: 1792-1802. Moore JD, Kleinfeld D, Wang F. 2014b. How the brainstem controls orofacial behaviors comprised of rhythmic actions. Trends in Neuroscience 27: 370-380. Moore JD, Deschênes M, Furuta T, Huber D, Smear MC, Demers M, Kleinfeld D. 2013. Hierarchy of orofacial rhythms revealed through whisking and breathing. Nature 469: 5357. Nguyen Q-T, Kleinfeld D. 2005. Positive feedback in a brainstem tactile sensorimotor loop. Neuron 45: 447-457. Ono T, Ishiwata Y, Inaba N, Kuroda T, Nakamura Y. 1998. Modulation of the inspiratory-related activity of hypoglossal premotor neurons during ingestion and rejection in the decerebrate cat. Journal of Neurophysiology 80: 48-58. Pietr MD, Knutsen PM, Shore DI, Ahissar E, Vogel Z. 2010. Cannabinoids reveal separate controls for whisking amplitude and timing in rats. Journal of Neurophysiology 104: 2532-2542. Pinganaud G, Bernat I, Buisseret P, Buisseret-Delmas C. 1999. Trigeminal projections to hypoglossal and facial motor nuclei in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 415: 91-104. Ranade S, Hangya B, Kepecs A. 2013. Multiple modes of phase locking between sniffing and whisking during active exploration. Journal of Neuroscience 33: 8250-8256. Rushworth MFS, Buckley MJ, Behrens TEJ, Walton WE, Bannerman DM. 2007. Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 17: 220-227. Sawczuk A, Mosier KM. 2001. Neural control of tongue movement with respect to respiration and swallowing. Critical Reviews in Oral Biology and Medicine 12: 18-37. Semba K, Komisaruk BR. 1984. Neural substrates of two different rhythmical vibrissal movements in the rat. Neuroscience 12: 761-774. Final draft: 2 January 2015 14 Sherrey JH, Megirian D. 1977. State dependence of upper airway respiratory motoneurons: Functions of the cricothyroid and nasolabial muscles of the unanesthetized rat. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 43: 218-228. Simony E, Bagdasarian K, Herfst L, Brecht M, Ahissar E, Golomb D. 2010. Temporal and spatial characteristics of vibrissa responses to motor commands. Journal of Neuroscience 30: 8935-8952. Smith JC, Abdala APL, Rybak IA, Paton JFR. 2009. Structural and functional architecture of respiratory networks in the mammalian brainstem. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 364: 2577-2587. Smith JC, Ellenberger HH, Ballanyi K, Richter DW, Feldman JL. 1991. Pre-Botzinger complex: A brainstem region that may generate respiratory rhythm in mammals. Science 254: 726-729. Stanek E, Cheng S, Takatoh J, Han BX, Wang F. 2014. Monosynaptic premotor circuit tracing reveals neural substrates for oro-motor coordination. Elife 3:e02511. Takatoh J, Nelson A, Zhou X, Bolton MM, Ehlers MD, Arenkiel BR, Mooney R, Wang F. 2013. New modules are added to vibrissal premotor circuitry with the emergence of exploratory whisking. Neuron 77: 346-360. Tan W, Janczewski WA, Yang P, Shao XM, Callaway EM, Feldman JL. 2008. Silencing preBötzinger complex somatostatin-expressing neurons induces persistent apnea in awake rat. Nature Neuroscience 11: 538 540. Tan W, Pagliardini S, Yang P, Janczewski WA, Feldman JL. 2010. Projections of preBötzinger complex neurons in adult rats. Journal of Comparative Neurology 18: 1862-1878. Tinbergen N. 1951. The Study of Instinct. Oxford University Press. Travers JB, DiNardo LA, H. K. 2000. Medullary reticular formation activity during ingestion and rejection in the awake rat. Experimental Brain Research 130: 78-92. Travers JB, Dinardo LA, Karimnamazi H. 1997. Motor and premotor mechanisms of licking. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 21: 631-647. Travers JB, Herman K, Travers SP. 2010. Supression of 3rd ventricular NPY-elicited feeding following medullary reticular formation infusion of muscimol. Behavior Neuroscience 124: 225-233. Travers JB, Yoo JE, Chandran R, Herman K, Travers SP. 2005. Neurotransmitter phenotypes of intermediate zone reticular formation projections to the motor trigeminal and hypoglossal nuclei in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 488: 28-47. Weiss P. 1941. Self-differentiation of the basic patterns of coordination. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore. Welker WI. 1964. Analysis of sniffing of the albino rat. Behaviour 12: 223-244. Welzl H, Bures J. 1977. Lick-synchronized breathing in rats. Physiological Behavior 18: 751753. Wickersham IR, Finke S, Conzelmann K-K, Callaway EM. 2007. Retrograde neuronal tracing with a deletion-mutant rabies virus. Nature Methods 4: 47-49. Wiesenfeld Z, Halpern BP, Tapper DN. 1977. Licking behavior: Evidence of hypoglossal oscillator. Science 196: 1122-1124. Wineski LE. 1985. Facial morphology and vibrissal movement in the golden hamster. Journal of Morphology 183: 199-217. Final draft: 2 January 2015

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

The Brainstem Oscillator for Whisking and the Case for Breathing as the Master Clock for Orofacial Motor Actions.

Whisking and sniffing are predominant aspects of exploratory behavior in rodents. We review evidence that these motor rhythms are coordinated by the respiratory patterning circuitry in the ventral medulla. A recently described region in the intermediate reticular zone of the medulla functions as an autonomous whisking oscillator, whose neuronal output is reset upon each breath by input from the...

متن کامل

Circuits in the Ventral Medulla That Phase-Lock Motoneurons for Coordinated Sniffing and Whisking

The exploratory behavior of rodents is characterized by stereotypical movements of the vibrissae, nose, and head, which are phase locked with rapid respiration, that is, sniffing. Here we review the brainstem circuitry that coordinates these actions and propose that respiration may act as a master clock for binding orofacial inputs across different sensory modalities.

متن کامل

The Central Pattern Generator for Rhythmic Whisking

Whisking and sniffing are predominant aspects of exploratory behavior in rodents. We review evidence that these motor rhythms are coordinated by the respiratory patterning circuitry in the ventral medulla. A region in the intermediate reticular zone, dorsomedial to the preBötzinger inspiratory complex, provides rhythmic input to the facial motoneurons that drive protraction of the vibrissae. Ne...

متن کامل

How the brainstem controls orofacial behaviors comprised of rhythmic actions.

Mammals perform a multitude of well-coordinated orofacial behaviors such as breathing, sniffing, chewing, licking, swallowing, vocalizing, and in rodents, whisking. The coordination of these actions must occur without fault to prevent fatal blockages of the airway. Deciphering the neuronal circuitry that controls even a single action requires understanding the integration of sensory feedback an...

متن کامل

Circuits in the rodent brainstem that control whisking in concert with other orofacial motor actions.

The world view of rodents is largely determined by sensation on two length scales. One is within the animal's peri-personal space; sensorimotor control on this scale involves active movements of the nose, tongue, head, and vibrissa, along with sniffing to determine olfactory clues. The second scale involves the detection of more distant space through vision and audition; these detection process...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2015